Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Hypnotic Clever Hands: Agency and Automatic Responding

2019

https://doi.org/10.1037/XGE0000451

Abstract

The Clever Hands task (Wegner, Fuller, & Sparrow, 2003) is a behavioral illusion in which participants make responses to a trivia quiz for which they have no sense of agency. Sixty high hypnotizable participants completed two versions of the Clever Hands task. Quiz one was a replication of the original study. Quiz two was a hypnotic adaptation using three suggestions that were based on clinical disruptions to the sense of agency. The suggestions were for: Random Responding, Thought Insertion, and Alien Control. These suggestions led to differences in accuracy (action production) and estimates of accuracy (action projection). Specifically, whereas the Random Responding suggestion had little effect, the two clinically based suggestions had opposite impacts on action production: the Thought Insertion suggestion led to an increase in the rate of correct responses (although participants still believed they were responding randomly); while the Alien Control suggestion led to a reduction i...

References (63)

  1. Bargh, J. A., & Thein, R. D. (1985). Individual construct accessibility, person memory, and the recall-judgment link: The case of information overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1129 -1146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.5.1129
  2. Barnier, A. J., Cox, R. E., & McConkey, K. M. (2014). The province of 'highs': The high hypnotizable person in the science of hypnosis and in psychological science. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 168 -183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cns0000018
  3. Barnier, A. J., Dienes, Z., & Mitchell, C. (2008). How hypnosis happens: New cognitive theories of hypnotic responding. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 141-177). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198570097.001.0001
  4. Barnier, A. J., & Oakley, D. A. (2009). Hypnosis and suggestion. In W. P. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopedia of consciousness (pp. 351-368). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012373873-8.00038-4
  5. Blakemore, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Wolpert, D. M. (1999). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 551-559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/ 089892999563607
  6. Bortolotti, L., & Broome, M. (2009). A role for ownership and authorship in the analysis of thought insertion. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8, 205-224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11097-008-9109-z
  7. Bowers, K. S. (1981). Do the Stanford Scales tap the "classic suggestion effect"? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 29, 42-53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207148108409142
  8. Bowers, K. S. (1990). Unconscious influences and hypnosis. In J. L. Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation: Implications for personality theory, psychopathology and health (pp. 143-179). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  9. Bowers, K. S., & Davidson, T. M. (1991). A neodissociative critique of Spanos's social-psychological model of hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp. 105-143). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Retrieved from http:// psycnet.apa.org/PsycINFO/1991-98913-004
  10. Bowers, P. (1982). The classic suggestion effect: Relationships with scales of hypnotizability, effortless experiencing, and imagery vividness. In- ternational Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 30, 270 - 279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207148208407264
  11. Camerer, C. F., & Hogarth, R. M. (1999). The effects of financial incen- tives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 7-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A: 1007850605129
  12. Connors, M. H., Barnier, A. J., Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2015). Hypnotic models of mirrored-self misidentification delusion: A review and an evaluation. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cns0000059
  13. Cox, R. E., & Barnier, A. J. (2010). Hypnotic illusions and clinical delusions: Hypnosis as a research method. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 202-232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546800903319884
  14. Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Proust, J., Pacherie, E., Dalery, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1997). Looking for the agent: An investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic pa- tients.
  15. Cognition, 65, 71-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97) 00039-5
  16. Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (2007). The cold control theory of hypnosis. In G. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neurosci- ence perspective (pp. 293-314). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  17. Egner, T., & Raz, A. (2007). Cognitive control processes and hypnosis. In G. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neu- roscience perspective (pp. 29 -50). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Ehrsson, H. H. (2007). The experimental induction of out-of-body expe- riences. Science, 317, 1048. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1142175
  18. Evans, F. J., & Graham, C. (1980). Subjective random number generation and attention deployment during acquisition and overlearning of a motor skill. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 391-394. http://dx.doi .org/10.3758/BF03334568
  19. Frith, C. (2005). The self in action: Lessons from delusions of control. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 14, 752-770. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.002
  20. Frith, C. D., & Done, D. J. (1989). Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action. Psychological Medicine, 19, 359 -363. https://doi.org/2762440. http://dx .doi.org/10.1017/S003329170001240X
  21. Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instru- ments & Computers, 35, 116 -124.
  22. Haggard, P., Cartledge, P., Dafydd, M., & Oakley, D. A. (2004). Anom- alous control: When 'free-will' is not conscious. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 13, 646 -654. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1016/j.concog.2004.06.001
  23. Haggard, P., & Tsakiris, M. (2009). The experience of agency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 242-246. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01644.x
  24. Hilgard, E. R. (1965). Hypnotic susceptibility. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & World.
  25. Hilgard, E. R. (1979). Divided consciousness in hypnosis: The implications of the hidden observer. In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor (Eds.), Hypnosis: Developments in research and new perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 45-79). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.
  26. Hilgard, E. R., Crawford, H. J., Bowers, P., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1979). A tailored SHSS:C, permitting user modification for special purposes. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 27, 125- 133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207147908407552
  27. Hur, J.-W., Kwon, J. S., Lee, T. Y., & Park, S. (2014). The crisis of minimal self-awareness in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review.
  28. Mellor, C. S. (1970). First rank symptoms of schizophrenia. I. The fre- quency in schizophrenics on admission to hospital. II. Differences be- tween individual first rank symptoms. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 117, 15-23.
  29. Metcalfe, J., & Greene, M. J. (2007). Metacognition of agency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 184 -199. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1037/0096-3445.136.2.184
  30. Mullins, S., & Spence, S. A. (2003). Re-examining thought insertion. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 293-298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/ bjp.182.4.293
  31. Neuringer, A. (1986). Can people behave 'randomly?': The role of feed- back. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 62-75. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.1.62
  32. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (pp. 1-18). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Retrieved from http://oai.dtic.mil/oai/ oai?verbϭgetRecord&metadataPrefixϭhtml&identifierϭADA094713
  33. Pacherie, E., Green, M., & Bayne, T. (2006). Phenomenology and delu- sions: Who put the 'alien' in alien control? Consciousness and Cogni- tion: An International Journal, 15, 566 -577. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.concog.2005.11.008
  34. Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., & Woody, E. Z. (2013). Developing the Sense of Agency Rating Scale (SOARS): An empirical measure of agency dis- ruption in hypnosis. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 22, 684 -696. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.003
  35. Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., Woody, E. Z., & Connors, M. H. (2014). Measuring agency change across the domain of hypnosis. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 3-19. http://dx.doi .org/10.1037/cns0000010
  36. Polito, V., Langdon, R., & Barnier, A. J. (2015). Sense of agency across contexts: Insights from schizophrenia and hypnosis. Psychology of Con- sciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2, 301-314. http://dx.doi .org/10.1037/cns0000053
  37. Pritchard, S. C., Zopf, R., Polito, V., Kaplan, D. M., & Williams, M. A. (2016). Non-hierarchical influence of visual form, touch, and position cues on embodiment, agency, and presence in virtual reality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1649. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01649
  38. Raz, A., & Campbell, N. K. J. (2011). Can suggestion obviate reading? Supplementing primary Stroop evidence with exploratory negative prim- ing analyses. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 20, 312-320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.013
  39. Raz, A., Kirsch, I., Pollard, J., & Nitkin-Kaner, Y. (2006). Suggestion reduces the stroop effect. Psychological Science, 17, 91-95. http://dx .doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01669.x
  40. Raz, A., Moreno-Iñiguez, M., Martin, L., & Zhu, H. (2007). Suggestion overrides the Stroop effect in highly hypnotizable individuals. Con- sciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 16, 331-338. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.04.004
  41. Raz, A., Shapiro, T., Fan, J., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Hypnotic suggestion and the modulation of Stroop interference. Archives of General Psychi- atry, 59, 1155-1161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.12.1155
  42. Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1962). The Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  43. Spanos, N. P. (1991). A sociocognitive approach to hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and per- spectives (pp. 324 -361). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  44. Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H. L., Hodgins, D. C., Stam, H. J., & Bertrand, L. D. (1983). The Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale: Normative data and psychometric properties. Psychological Reports, 53, 523-535. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1983.53.2.523
  45. Spence, S. (2001). Alien control: From phenomenology to cognitive neu- robiology. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 8, 163-172. http://dx .doi.org/10.1353/ppp.2001.0017
  46. Steiger, J. H. (2004). Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. Psychological Methods, 9, 164 -182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082- 989X.9.2.164
  47. Stephens, G. L., & Graham, G. (1994). Self-consciousness, mental agency, and the clinical psychopathology of thought insertion. Philosophy, Psy- chiatry, & Psychology, 1, 1-10.
  48. Terhune, D. B., Cleeremans, A., Raz, A., & Lynn, S. J. (2017). Hypnosis and top-down regulation of consciousness. Neuroscience and Biobehav- ioral Reviews, 81, 59 -74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02 .002 van der Weiden, A., Ruys, K. I., & Aarts, H. (2013). A matter of matching: How goals and primes affect self-agency experiences. Journal of Ex- This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. perimental Psychology: General, 142, 954 -966. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1037/a0030079
  49. Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2007). Thoughts, motor actions, and the self. Mind & Language, 22, 22-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017 .2006.00298.x
  50. Voss, M., Moore, J., Hauser, M., Gallinat, J., Heinz, A., & Haggard, P. (2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: A specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 133, 3104 -3112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq152
  51. Walsh, E., Mehta, M. A., Oakley, D. A., Guilmette, D. N., Gabay, A., Halligan, P. W., & Deeley, Q. (2014). Using suggestion to model different types of automatic writing. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 26, 24 -36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog .2014.02.008
  52. Walsh, E., Oakley, D. A., Halligan, P. W., Mehta, M. A., & Deeley, Q. (2015). The functional anatomy and connectivity of thought insertion and alien control of movement. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 64, 380 -393. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1016/j.cortex.2014.09.012
  53. Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.
  54. Wegner, D. M. (2009). How to think, say, or do precisely the worst thing for any occasion. Science, 325, 48 -50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science .1167346
  55. Wegner, D. M., Fuller, V. A., & Sparrow, B. (2003). Clever hands: Uncontrolled intelligence in facilitated communication. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 85, 5-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0022-3514.85.1.5
  56. Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480 -492. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.480
  57. Weitzenhoffer, A. M. (1974). When is an "instruction" an "instruction"? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 22, 258 - 269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207147408413005
  58. Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Stanford Hypnotic Sus- ceptibility Scale, Form C. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  59. Wilson, S. C., & Barber, T. X. (1978). The creative imagination scale as a measure of hypnotic responsiveness: Applications to experimental and clinical hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 20, 235-249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00029157.1978.10403940
  60. Woody, E. Z., Barnier, A. J., & McConkey, K. M. (2005). Multiple hypnotizabilities: Differentiating the building blocks of hypnotic re- sponse. Psychological Assessment, 17, 200 -211. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1037/1040-3590.17.2.200
  61. Woody, E. Z., & Bowers, K. S. (1994). A frontal assault on dissociated control. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives (pp. 52-79). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  62. Woody, E. Z., & McConkey, K. M. (2003). What we don't know about the brain and hypnosis, but need to: A view from the Buckhorn Inn. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51, 309 - 338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/iceh.51.3.309.15523
  63. Woody, E. Z., & Sadler, P. (2008). Dissociation theories of hypnosis. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 81-110). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198570097 .013.0004