Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Measuring agency change across the domain of hypnosis

2014, Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice

https://doi.org/10.1037/CNS0000010
descriptionSee full PDF

Abstract

Building on classic work, the domain of hypnosis has been conceptualized by as comprising 3 levels that represent distinct aspects of hypnotic phenomena: a) responses to different types of hypnotic suggestions, b) varying patterns of response over the phases of a suggestion, and c) the impact of state and trait influences. The current experiment investigates sense of agency across each of these three levels. Forty-six high hypnotizable participants completed an ideomotor (arm levitation), a challenge (arm rigidity), and a cognitive (anosmia) item, with or without a hypnotic induction. In a postexperimental inquiry, participants rated their feelings of control at three time points for each item: during the suggestion, test, and cancellation phases. They also completed the Sense of Agency Rating Scale (Polito, Barnier, & Woody, 2013) for each item. Pass rates and control ratings fluctuated across the different types of items and the three phases of each item; control ratings and agency scores also differed between participants who passed and failed each item. In addition, whereas a hypnotic induction influenced the likelihood of passing items, it had no direct effect on agentive experiences. These results suggest that altered sense of agency is not a unidimensional or static quality "switched on" by hypnotic induction, but a dynamic multidimensional construct that varies across items, over time, and according to whether individuals pass or fail suggestions.

References (68)

  1. Arbuckle, J. L. (2012). IBM SPSS Amos 21 user's guide. Chicago, IL: Amos Development Corpora- tion.
  2. Balthazard, C. G., & Woody, E. Z. (1985). The "stuff" of hypnotic performance: A review of psy- chometric approaches. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 283-296. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.283
  3. Barber, T. X. (1999). A comprehensive three- dimensional theory of hypnosis. In I. Kirsch, A. Capafons, E. Cardeña-Buelna, & S. Amigo (Eds.), Clinical hypnosis and self-regulation: Cognitive- behavioral perspectives (Dissociation, trauma, memory, and hypnosis book series; pp. 21-48). Washington, DC: American Psychological Associ- ation. doi:10.1037/10282-001
  4. Barnier, A. J., Dienes, Z., & Mitchell, C. (2008). How hypnosis happens: New cognitive theories of hypnotic responding. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 141-177). Ox- ford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/ oxfordhb/9780198570097.013.0006
  5. Benham, G., Woody, E. Z., Wilson, K. S., & Nash, M. R. (2006). Expect the unexpected: Ability, at- titude, and responsiveness to hypnosis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 342-350. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.2.342
  6. Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 237-242. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1
  7. Bortolotti, L., & Broome, M. (2009). A role for ownership and authorship in the analysis of thought insertion. Phenomenology and the Cogni- tive Sciences, 8, 205-224. doi:10.1007/s11097- 008-9109-z
  8. Bowers, K. S. (1981). Do the Stanford scales tap the "classic suggestion effect"? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 29, 42-53. doi:10.1080/00207148108409142
  9. Bowers, P. (1982). The classic suggestion effect: Rela- tionships with scales of hypnotizability, effortless experiencing, and imagery vividness. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 30, 270. doi:10.1080/00207148208407264
  10. Bowers, P., Laurence, J.-R., & Hart, D. (1988). The experience of hypnotic suggestions. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 36, 336 -349. doi:10.1080/00207148808410523
  11. Brown, R. J., & Oakley, D. A. (2004). An integrative cognitive theory of hypnosis and high hypnotiz- ability. In M. Heap, R. J. Brown, & D. A. Oakley (Eds.), The highly hypnotizable person: Theoreti- cal, experimental and clinical issues (pp. 152- 186). London, UK: Brunner-Routledge.
  12. Cardeña, E. (2005). The phenomenology of deep hyp- nosis: Quiescent and physically active. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 53, 37-59. doi:10.1080/00207140490914234
  13. Carruthers, G. (2012). A metacognitive model of the sense of agency over thoughts. Cognitive Neuro- psychiatry, 17, 291-314. doi:10.1080/13546805 .2011.627275
  14. Comey, G., & Kirsch, I. (1999). Intentional and spon- taneous imagery in hypnosis: The phenomenology of hypnotic responding. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 47, 65-85. doi:10.1080/00207149908410023
  15. Field, P. B. (1966). Some self-rating measures related to hypnotizability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23, 1179 -1187. doi:10.2466/pms.1966.23.3f.1179
  16. Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and con- trol of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sci- ences, 355, 1771-1788. doi:10.1098/rstb.2000 .0734
  17. Galea, V., Woody, E. Z., Szechtman, H., & Pier- rynowski, M. R. (2010). Motion in response to the hypnotic suggestion of arm rigidity: A window on underlying mechanisms. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 58, 251-268. doi:10.1080/00207141003760561
  18. Gruzelier, J. (2005). Altered states of consciousness and hypnosis in the twenty-first century. Contem- porary Hypnosis, 22, 1-7. doi:10.1002/ch.14
  19. Hilgard, E. R. (1965). Hypnotic susceptibility. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & World.
  20. Hilgard, E. R. (1973). The domain of hypnosis. With some comments on alternative paradigms. Ameri- can Psychologist, 28, 972-982. doi:10.1037/ h0035452
  21. Hilgard, E. R. (1979). Divided consciousness in hyp- nosis: The implications of the hidden observer. In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor (Eds.), Hypnosis: Devel- opments in research and new perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 45-79). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.
  22. Hilgard, E. R., & Tart, C. T. (1966). Responsiveness to suggestions following waking and imagination instructions and following induction of hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 71, 196 -208. doi:10.1037/h0023323
  23. Kallio, S., Hyönä, J., Revonsuo, A., Sikka, P., & Nummenmaa, L. (2011). The existence of a hyp- notic state revealed by eye movements. PLoS ONE, 6, e26374. doi:10.1371/journal.pone .0026374
  24. Kihlstrom, J. F. (1985). Hypnosis. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 385-418. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps .36.020185.002125
  25. Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Measurement of involuntari- ness in hypnotic response. Retrieved March 11, 2012, from http://ist-socrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/ PDFfiles/Hypnotizability/HGSHSAResponse1002 .pdf Kihlstrom, J. F. (2008). The domain of hypnosis, revisited. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, re- search and practice (pp. 21-52). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/ 9780198570097.013.0002
  26. Kirsch, I. (2011). The altered state issue: Dead or alive? International Journal of Clinical and Ex- perimental Hypnosis, 59, 350. doi:10.1080/ 00207144.2011.570681
  27. Kirsch, I., Mazzoni, G., & Montgomery, G. H. (2007). Remembrance of hypnosis past. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 49, 171-178. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2007.10401574
  28. Kosslyn, S. M. (2000). Hypnotic visual illusion alters color processing in the brain. The American Jour- nal of Psychiatry, 157, 1279 -1284. doi:10.1176/ appi.ajp.157.8.1279
  29. Laurence, J.-R., & Nadon, R. (1986). Reports of hypnotic depth: Are they more than mere words? International Journal of Clinical and Experi- mental Hypnosis, 34, 215-233. doi:10.1080/ 00207148608406987
  30. Lynn, S. J. (1997). Automaticity and hypnosis: A sociocognitive account. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 45, 239 -250. doi:10.1080/00207149708416126
  31. Lynn, S. J., Fassler, O., & Knox, J. (2005). Hypnosis and the altered state debate: Something more or nothing more? Contemporary Hypnosis, 22, 39 - 45. doi:10.1002/ch.21
  32. Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I., & Hallquist, M. N. (2008). Sociocognitive theories of hypnosis. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/ oxfordhb/9780198570097.013.0005
  33. Lynn, S. J., Nash, M. R., Rhue, J. W., Frauman, D., & Stanley, S. (1983). Hypnosis and the experience of nonvolition. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 31, 293-308. doi: 10.1080/00207148308406624
  34. McConkey, K. M. (1991). The construction and res- olution of experience and behaviour in hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp. 542-563). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
  35. McConkey, K. M. (2008). Generations and landscape of hypnosis: Questions we've asked, questions we should ask. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, re- search and practice (pp. 53-77). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/ 9780198570097.013.0003
  36. McConkey, K. M., & Barnier, A. J. (2004). High hypnotizability: Unity and diversity in behavior and experience. In M. Heap, R. J. Brown, & D. A. Oakley (Eds.), The highly hypnotizable person: Theoretical, experimental and clinical issues (pp. 61-84). New York, NY: Routledge.
  37. McConkey, K. M., Sheehan, P. W., & Law, H. G. (1980). Structural analysis of the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. Interna- tional Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypno- sis, 28, 164 -175. doi:10.1080/00207148008409838
  38. McConkey, K. M., Szeps, A., & Barnier, A. J. (2001). Indexing the experience of sex change in hypnosis and imagination. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 49, 123-138. doi:10.1080/00207140108410063
  39. McConkey, K. M., Wende, V., & Barnier, A. J. (1999). Measuring change in the subjective expe- rience of hypnosis. International Journal of Clin- ical and Experimental Hypnosis, 47, 23. doi: 10.1080/00207149908410020
  40. Meyer, E. C., & Lynn, S. J. (2011). Responding to hypnotic and nonhypnotic suggestions: Perfor- mance standards, imaginative suggestibility, and response expectancies. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 59, 327-349. doi:10.1080/00207144.2011.570660
  41. Mullins, S., & Spence, S. (2003). Re-examining thought insertion: Semi-structured literature re- view and conceptual analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 293-298. doi:10.1192/bjp.182 .4.293
  42. Nash, M. R. (2005). The importance of being earnest when crafting definitions: Science and scientism are not the same thing. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 53, 265-280. doi:10.1080/00207140590961934
  43. Nogrady, H., McConkey, K. M., & Perry, C. (1985). Enhancing visual memory: Trying hypnosis, trying imagination, and trying again. Journal of Abnor- mal Psychology, 94, 195-204. doi:10.1037/0021- 843X.94.2.195
  44. Orne, M. T. (1967). What must a satisfactory theory of hypnosis explain? International journal of psy- chiatry, 3, 206 -211.
  45. Orne, M. T. (1979). On the simulating subject as a quasi-control group in hypnosis research: What, why, and how. In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor (Eds.), Hypnosis: Developments in research and new per- spectives (pp. 399 -444). New York, NY: Aldine.
  46. Pekala, R. J. (1991). Quantifying consciousness: An empirical approach. New York, NY: Plenum Press. doi:10.1007/978-1-4899-0629-8
  47. Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., & McConkey, K. M. (2014). Defining hypnosis: Process, product, and the value of tolerating ambiguity [Commentary on Wagstaff's "On the centrality of the concept of an altered state to definitions of hypnosis"]. Journal of Mind-Body Regulation, 2, 169 -171.
  48. Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., & Woody, E. Z. (2013). Developing the Sense of Agency Rating Scale (SOARS): An empirical measure of agency dis- ruption in hypnosis. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 684 -696. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.003
  49. Ray, W. J., & Pascalis, V. (2003). Temporal aspects of hypnotic processes. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51, 147-165. doi:10.1076/iceh.51.2.147.14615
  50. Sarbin, T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1972). Hypnosis: A social psychological analysis of influence commu- nication. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Win- ston.
  51. Sheehan, P. W., & McConkey, K. M. (1982). Hyp- nosis and experience: The exploration of phenom- ena and process. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  52. Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1962). The Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  53. Spanos, N. P. (1991). A sociocognitive approach to hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and per- spectives (pp. 324 -361). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  54. Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H. L., Hodgins, D. C., Stam, H. J., & Bertrand, L. D. (1983). The Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale: Normative data and psychometric properties. Psy- chological Reports, 53, 523-535. doi:10.2466/pr0 .1983.53.2.523
  55. Stephens, G. L., & Graham, G. (1994). Self- consciousness, mental agency, and the clinical psychopathology of thought insertion. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 1, 1-10.
  56. Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2007). Thoughts, motor actions, and the self. Mind & Language, 22, 22- 43. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.2006.00298.x
  57. Wechsler, D. (1997). Manual of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
  58. Wegner, D. M. (2004). Précis of the illusion of conscious will. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 649 -659. doi:10.1017/S0140525X04000159
  59. Weitzenhoffer, A. M. (1974). When is an "instruc- tion" an "instruction?" International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 22, 258 -269. doi:10.1080/00207147408413005
  60. Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Stan- ford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  61. Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1963). Stan- ford Profile Scales of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Forms I and II. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psy- chologists Press.
  62. White, R. W. (1941). A preface to the theory of hypnotism. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 36, 477. doi:10.1037/h0053844
  63. Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor inte- gration. Science, 269, 1880 -1882. doi:10.1126/ science.7569931
  64. Woody, E. Z., & Barnier, A. J. (2008). Hypnosis scales for the twenty-first century: What do we need and how should we use them? In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 255- 280). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198570097.013.0010
  65. Woody, E. Z., Barnier, A. J., & McConkey, K. M. (2005). Multiple hypnotizabilities: Differentiating the building blocks of hypnotic response. Psycho- logical Assessment, 17, 200 -211. doi:10.1037/ 1040-3590.17.2.200
  66. Woody, E. Z., & McConkey, K. M. (2003). What we don't know about the brain and hypnosis, but need to: A view from the buckhorn inn. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51, 309 -338. doi:10.1076/iceh.51.3.309.15523
  67. Woody, E. Z., & Sadler, P. (2008). Dissociation theories of hypnosis. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice. Oxford, UK: Ox- ford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/ 9780198570097.013.0004
  68. Revision received January 13, 2014 Accepted January 14, 2014 Ⅲ