Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Variations in the sense of agency during hypnotic responding: Insights from latent profile analysis

2016, Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice

https://doi.org/10.1037/CNS0000107
descriptionSee full PDF

Abstract

The primary phenomenological feature of a response to hypnotic suggestion is the perception that a person is not the author of their actions and experiences. This distortion in volition during hypnotic responding, known as the classic suggestion effect, has the potential to illuminate the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying hypnosis and inform broader models of agency. Here we sought to clarify inter-individual differences in the patterns of agency that participants experience during hypnosis. We applied latent profile analysis, a finite mixture modeling method for partitioning participants into homogeneous classes, to participants' responses to a standardized behavioral measure of hypnotic suggestibility and an experiential measure of sense of agency during hypnotic responding. The best fitting model suggested that there were four discrete response patterns: a low suggestible class, two medium suggestible classes, and one highly suggestible class. The two medium suggestible classes displayed nearly equivalent patterns of behavioural hypnotic responding but diverged in their experience of agency during hypnotic responding: one class experienced greater involuntariness during responding whereas the other experienced greater effortlessness during responding. These results reinforce previous research highlighting differential patterns of hypnotic responding and complement work suggesting that there may be two or more phenomenologically distinct modes of hypnotic responding. They also have a number of implications for the measurement of hypnotic responding and for the use of low and medium suggestible individuals in experimental hypnosis research designs.

References (52)

  1. Barnier, A. J., Cox, R. E., & McConkey, K. M. (2014). The province of "highs": The high hypnotizable person in the science of hypnosis and in psychological science. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(2), 168-183.
  2. Barnier, A. J., & McConkey, K. M. (2003). Hypnosis, human nature, and complexity: Integrating neuroscience approaches into hypnosis research. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51, 282-308.
  3. Barnier, A. J., & McConkey, K. M. (2004). Defining and identifying the highly hypnotizable person. In M. Heap, R. J. Brown & D. A. Oakley (Eds.), The highly hypnotizable person: Theoretical, experimental and clinical issues (pp. 30-60). London, UK: Brunner-Routledge.
  4. Bowers, K. S. (1981). Do the Stanford Scales tap the "classic suggestion effect"? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 29(1), 42-53. doi: 10.1080/00207148108409142
  5. Bowers, P. (1982). The classic suggestion effect: Relationships with scales of hypnotizability, effortless experiencing, and imagery vividness. Int J Clin Exp Hypn, 30(3), 270-279.
  6. Bowers, P., Laurence, J. R., & Hart, D. (1988). The experience of hypnotic suggestions. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 36(4), 336-349. doi: 10.1080/00207148808410523
  7. Brown, R. J., Antonova, E., Langley, A., & Oakley, D. A. (2001). The effects of absorption and reduced critical though on suggestibility in an hypnotic context. Contemporary hypnosis, 18, 62-72.
  8. Brown, R. J., & Oakley, D. A. (1998). Hypnotic susceptibility and holistic/emotional styles of thinking. Contemporary hypnosis, 15, 76-83.
  9. Burgess, C. A., Kirsch, I., Shane, H., Niederauer, K. L., Graham, S. M., & Bacon, A. (1998). Facilitated communication as an ideomotor response. Psychological Science, 9(1), 71-74. doi: Doi 10.1111/1467-9280.00013
  10. Cardeña, E., & Terhune, D. B. (2014). Hypnotizability, personality traits and the propensity to experience alterations of consciousness. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 292- 307.
  11. Dienes, Z., & Hutton, S. (2013). Understanding hypnosis metacognitively: rTMS applied to left DLPFC increases hypnotic suggestibility. Cortex, 49(2), 386-392. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.07.009
  12. Efron, B. (1987). Better bootstrap confidence intervals. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 82, 171-185.
  13. Galea, V., Woody, E. Z., Szechtman, H., & Pierrynowski, M. R. (2010). Motion in response to the hypnotic suggestion of arm rigidity: A window on underlying mechanisms. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 58(3), 251-268. doi: 10.1080/00207141003760561
  14. Gorassini, D. R. (2004). Enhancing hypnotizability The highly hypnotizable person: Theoretical, experimental and clinical issues (pp. 213-239). New York, NY: Routledge.
  15. Haggard, P., Cartledge, P., Dafydd, M., & Oakley, D. A. (2004). Anomalous control: When 'free-will' is not conscious. Conscious Cogn, 13(3), 646-654.
  16. Jamieson, G. A., & Sheehan, P. W. (2004). An empirical test of Woody and Bowers's dissociated-control theory of hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 52(3), 232-249.
  17. Kihlstrom, J. F. (2008). The domain of hypnosis, revisited. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis (pp. 21-52). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  18. King, B. J., & Council, J. R. (1998). Intentionality during hypnosis: An ironic process analysis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 46(3), 295-313.
  19. Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I., & Hallquist, M. (2008). Social cognitive theories of hypnosis. In M. R. Nash & A. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 111-140). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  20. Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I., Knox, J., Fassler, O., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Hypnosis and neuroscience: Implications for the altered state debate. In G. A. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective (pp. 145-165). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  21. McConkey, K. M., & Barnier, A. J. (2004). High hypnotizability: Unity and diversity in behavior and experience. In M. Heap, R. J. Brown & D. A. Oakley (Eds.), The highly hypnotizable person: Theoretical, experimental and clinical issues (pp. 61-84). New York, NY: Routledge.
  22. McConkey, K. M., Glisky, M. L., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1989). Individual differences among hypnotic virtuosos: A case comparison. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 17, 131- 140.
  23. McCutcheon, A. C. (1987). Latent class analysis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
  24. McLachlan, G., & Peel, D. (2000). Finite mixture models. New York, NY: Wiley.
  25. Metcalfe, J., Van Snellenberg, J. X., DeRosse, P., Balsam, P., & Malhotra, A. K. (2012). Judgements of agency in schizophrenia: An impairment in autonoetic metacognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1391-1400. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0006
  26. Montgomery, G. H., Schnur, J. B., & David, D. (2011). The impact of hypnotic suggestibility in clinical care settings. Int J Clin Exp Hypn, 59(3), 294-309. doi: 10.1080/00207144.2011.570656
  27. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
  28. Newberg, A. B., Wintering, N. A., Morgan, D., & Waldman, M. R. (2006). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during glossolalia: A preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research, 148(1), 67-71. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.07.001
  29. Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 535-569.
  30. Pekala, R. J., & Kumar, V. K. (2007). An empirical-phenomenological approach to quantifying consciousness and states of consciousness: With particular reference to understanding the nature of hypnosis. In G. A. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective (pp. 167-194). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  31. Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., & Woody, E. Z. (2013). Developing the Sense of Agency Rating Scale (SOARS): An empirical measure of agency disruption in hypnosis. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 684- 696. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.003
  32. Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., Woody, E. Z., & Connors, M. H. (2014). Measuring agency change across the domain of hypnosis. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(1), 3-19.
  33. Polito, V., Langdon, R., & Barnier, A. J. (2015). Sense of agency across contexts: Insights from schizophrenia and hypnosis. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2(3), 301-314.
  34. Ramaswamy, V., Desarbo, W. S., Reibstein, D. J., & Robinson, W. T. (1993). An empirical pooling approach for estimating marketing mix elasticities with PIMS data Marketing Science, 12, 103-124.
  35. Schwartz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461-464.
  36. Sheehan, P. W., & McConkey, K. M. (1982). Hypnosis and experience: The exploration of phenomena and process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  37. Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1962). Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  38. Spiegel, D., Lewis-Fernandez, R., Lanius, R., Vermetten, E., Simeon, D., & Friedman, M. (2013). Dissociative disorders in DSM-5. Annual review of clinical psychology, 9, 299-326. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185531
  39. Terhune, D. B. (2015). Discrete response patterns in the upper range of hypnotic suggestibility: A latent profile analysis. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 334-341. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.018
  40. Terhune, D. B., & Cardeña, E. (2010). Differential patterns of spontaneous experiential response to a hypnotic induction: A latent profile analysis. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 1140-1150. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.006
  41. Terhune, D. B., & Cardeña, E. (2015). Dissociative subtypes in posttraumatic stress disorders and hypnosis: Neurocognitive parallels and clinical implications. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 452-457.
  42. Terhune, D. B., Cardeña, E., & Lindgren, M. (2011a). Dissociated control as a signature of typological variability in high hypnotic suggestibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 727-736. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.11.005
  43. Terhune, D. B., Cardeña, E., & Lindgren, M. (2011b). Dissociative tendencies and individual differences in high hypnotic suggestibility. Cogn Neuropsychiatry, 16(2), 113-135. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2010.503048
  44. Vermunt, J. K., & Magidson, J. (2002). Latent class cluster analysis. In J. A. Hagenaars & A. L. McCutcheon (Eds.), Applied latent class analysis (pp. 89-106). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  45. Wegner, D. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  46. Weitzenhoffer, A. M. (1974). When is an "instruction" an "instruction"? Int J Clin Exp Hypn, 22(3), 258- 269. doi: 10.1080/00207147408413005
  47. Weitzenhoffer, A. M. (1980). Hypnotic susceptibility revisited. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 22(3), 130-146.
  48. Whalley, M. G., & Brooks, G. B. (2009). Enhancement of suggestibility and imaginative ability with nitrous oxide. Psychopharmacology, 203(4), 745-752.
  49. Winkel, J. D., Younger, J. W., Tomcik, N., Borckardt, J. J., & Nash, M. R. (2006). Anatomy of a hypnotic response: Self-report estimates, actual behavior, and physiological response to the hypnotic suggestion of arm rigidity. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 54, 186- 205.
  50. Woody, E., & Szechtman, H. (2007). To see feelingly: Emotion, motivation and hypnosis. In G. A. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective (pp. 241-255 ). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  51. Woody, E. Z., & Barnier, A. J. (2008). Hypnosis scales for the twenty-first century: What do we know and how should we use them? In M. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research and practice (pp. 255-281). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  52. Woody, E. Z., & Szechtman, H. (2003). How can brain activity and hypnosis inform each other? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51, 232-255.